FOCUS DC News Wire 4/16/2015

Friends of Choice in Urban Schools (FOCUS) is now the DC Charter School Alliance!

Please visit www.dccharters.org to learn about our new organization and to see the latest news and information related to DC charter schools.

The FOCUS DC website is online to see historic information, but is not actively updated.

NEWS

What Drives Public-School Demand? Location, Location, Location.
Washington City Paper
By Aaron Wiener
April 15, 2015

Last week, the District released the waitlist numbers for each public and charter school. For D.C. Public Schools, this means the number of students who couldn't get in on the first round to an out-of-boundary school or to a pre-kindergarten program. For charters, it's the number who couldn't get into a school in the citywide lottery, since there's no neighborhood preference for charters.

The map above shows the results, courtesy of the District, Measured blog from the Office of Revenue Analysis. Orange circles represent traditional public schools; blue circles represent charters. The larger the circle, the longer the waitlist.

The pattern for traditional public schools is striking. Nearly every public school with a long waitlist is west of Rock Creek Park or on Capitol Hill. The trend among charters is less obvious, since there are no charters west of Rock Creek Park. But all the longest waitlists are still west of the Anacostia River, although there are plenty of charters to the east of the river.

Even when two schools have equal academic performance, there's a sharp geographic disparity in demand for the schools. Take Capitol Hill Montessori at Logan and Shepherd Elementary School. Both are DCPS elementary schools with 73 percent reading proficiency. But Capitol Hill Montessori, near Union Station, has a waitlist of 716 students, while Shepherd, in Shepherd Park, has a waitlist half that size, with 394 students.

Or take two lower-performing elementary schools: Bancroft Elementary School in Mount Pleasant and Martin Luther King Elementary School in Congress Heights. King actually has slightly higher reading proficiency: 32 percent to Bancroft's 31. But Bancroft's waitlist is 460 names long, while King has no waitlist at all. Why? In part, it's surely because Bancroft feeds into well-regarded Deal Middle School and Wilson High School, while King feeds into lower-performing Hart Middle School and Ballou High School.

Perhaps the biggest outlier here is Benjamin Banneker High School. The application-only school has consistently had among the highest test scores of any in the city. And yet, perhaps because of its existing demographics, many parents continue to shy away from it. This year, the school is 81 percent black, 14 percent Hispanic, and zero percent white, despite the growing number of white families in the surrounding neighborhoods, like Columbia Heights and Logan Circle and LeDroit Park.

These numbers paint a disheartening picture. City officials' mantra has been that if they enhance the academic performance of schools in poorer parts of the District, they can bring more equity to the public-school system and stop the closure of schools in lower-income neighborhoods due to under-enrollment. But it appears that the correlation between performance and demand is messy, and families still have a strong preference for schools in certain neighborhoods.

New brain science shows poor kids have smaller brains than affluent kids
The Washington Post
By Lyndsey Layton
April 15, 2015

New research that shows poor children have smaller brains than affluent children has deepened the national debate about ways to narrow the achievement gap.

Neuroscientists who studied the brain scans of nearly
1,100 children and young adults nationwide from ages 3 to 20 found that the surface area of the cerebral cortex was linked to family income. They discovered that the brains of children in families that earned less than $25,000 a year had surface areas 6 percent smaller than those whose families earned $150,000 or more. The poor children also scored lower on average on a battery of cognitive tests.

The region of the brain in question handles language, memory, spatial skills and reasoning, all important to success in school and beyond.

The study, published last month in Nature Neuroscience, is the largest of its kind to date. It was led by Kimberly Noble, who teaches at both Columbia University’s Teachers College and the university’s medical school. Elizabeth Sowell, of Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, was the senior author.

“We’ve known for so long that poverty and lack of access to resources to enrich the developmental environment are related to poor school performance, poor test scores and fewer educational opportunities,” Sowell said. “But now we can really tie it to a physical thing in the brain. We realized that this is a big deal.”

The study is part of a new and growing body of research on children’s brain structures that has been made possible by technological advances in magnetic resonance imaging.

“It’s only been in the past
20 years that we could have done this with living, developing children,” said Sowell, who published a pioneering 1999 study that found the brain is still developing past adolescence, contrary to earlier beliefs that brain growth was complete by the teen years.

The research comes at a time when a majority of U.S. public school students come from low-income families and the academic achievement gap between poor and more-affluent children is growing. Policymakers are increasingly concerned about ways to reduce the gap, which is apparent as early as kindergarten.

In another study that has been accepted for publication in Psychological Science, a team led by neuroscientist John Gabrieli of MIT found differences in the brain’s cortical thickness between low-income and higher-income teenagers. The study linked that difference for the first time to standardized test scores: Fifty-seven percent of the poor children scored proficient in math and reading tests given annually in Massachusetts, compared with 91 percent of the higher-income students.

“The thing that really stands out is how powerful the economic influences are on something as fundamental as brain structure,” Gabrieli said. “It’s just very striking.”

The new research does not explain possible reasons for the brain differences. And that has created concern that the findings will harden stereotypes and give an impression that children who are born into poverty lack the physical capacity to succeed academically.

“Some people feel if you show these brain differences, you’re politically condemning the poor,” Gabrieli said. “Which is the opposite, I think, of what we need to do. I think we want to understand adversity and minimize adversity.”

Noble and Sowell have two theories about why poor children have smaller brains. One is that poor families lack access to material goods that aid healthy development, such as good nutrition and higher-quality health care. The other is that poor families tend to live more chaotic lives, and that stress could inhibit healthy brain development.

Noble has embarked on a new study to try to answer that question. She has begun a pilot study to investigate whether giving low-income mothers a small or large monthly sum of cash impacts the cognitive development of their children in the first three years of life. She plans to recruit 1,000 low-income mothers from around the country, half of whom would receive $333 a month, while the other half would receive $20 a month for three years. That research is expected to take five years.

But James Thompson, a psychologist at University College London, has a third theory.

“People who have less ability and marry people with less ability have children who, on balance, on average, have less ability,” he said. Thompson noted that there is a genetic component to intelligence that Noble and Sowell failed to consider.

“It makes my jaw drop that we’ve known for years intelligence is inheritable and scientists are beginning to track down exactly how it happens,” Thompson said. “The well-known genetic hypothesis has not even had a chance to enter the door in this discussion.”

Charles Murray, a conservative political scientist who argues there is a relationship between intelligence and economic class in his book “The Bell Curve,” said genetics cannot be ignored.

“It is confidently known that brain size is correlated with IQ, IQ measured in childhood is correlated with income as an adult, and parental IQ is correlated with children’s IQ,” Murray wrote in an e-mail. “I would be astonished if children’s brain size were NOT correlated with parental income. How could it be otherwise?”

In releasing their study, Noble and Sowell emphasized that the brain can grow and change. “That is a very critical point,” Noble said. “The brain is in­cred­ibly able to be molded by experience, especially in childhood.”

Mike Feinberg, a co-founder of the Knowledge is Power Program (KIPP), a network of 162 charter schools in 20 states and the District of Columbia that educates 59,000 students, 85 percent of whom have low family income, said all children are capable of learning regardless of their backgrounds or economic situation.

“We’ve been in the business of growing brains for 20 years now,” Feinberg said. “For the vast majority of children, there is nothing physically about them that sets them up for success or failure as they start school. There are certainly societal circumstances that make it easier or harder for that child to learn on any given day. And certainly, children in poverty are going to develop more physical issues as well if they’re not taken care of. But are they able to learn? Absolutely.”

The Obama administration has increasingly promoted the idea that the country should provide early childhood education for low-income 3- and 4-year-olds to give them a boost before they get to kindergarten. Last week, Education Secretary Arne Duncan said if he had one more federal dollar to spend on education, he would funnel it to early childhood.

__________

 

Mailing Archive: